1887
banner image
No data available.
Please log in to see this content.
You have no subscription access to this content.
No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
oa
Lateralization produced by interaural intensitive disparities appears to be larger for high- vs low-frequency stimuli
Rent:
Rent this article for
Access full text Article
/content/asa/journal/jasa/129/1/10.1121/1.3528756
1.
1. Bernstein, L. R. , and Trahiotis, C. (1985). “Lateralization of low-frequency, complex waveforms: The use of envelope-based temporal disparities,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 77, 18681880.
2.
2. Bernstein, L. R. , and Trahiotis, C. (1994). “Detection of interaural delay in high-frequency SAM tones, two-tone complexes, and bands of noise,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95, 35613567.
3.
3. Bernstein, L. R. , and Trahiotis, C. (2003). “Enhancing interaural-delay-based extents of laterality at high frequencies by using ‘transposed stimuli’,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 33353347.
4.
4. Bernstein, L. R. , and Trahiotis, C. (2009). “How sensitivity to ongoing interaural temporal disparities is affected by manipulations of temporal features of the envelopes of high-frequency stimuli,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125, 32343242.
5.
6. Buell, T. N. , Trahiotis, C. , and Bernstein, L. R. (1991). “Lateralization of low-frequency tones: Relative potency of gating and ongoing interaural delay,” J. Acoust. Soc Am. 90, 30773085.
6.
7. Domnitz, R. H. , and Colburn, H. S. (1977). “Lateral position and interaural discrimination,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 61, 15861598.
7.
8. Grantham, D. W. (1984). “Interaural intensity discrimination: Insensitivity at 1000 Hz,” J. Acoust. Soc Am. 75, 11911194.
8.
9. Heller, L. M. , and Trahiotis, C. (1996). “Extents of laterality and binaural interference effects,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99, 36323637.
9.
10. Stern, R. M. , and Colburn, H. S. (1978). “Theory of binaural interaction based on auditory-nerve data. IV. A model for subjective lateral position,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 64, 127140.
10.
11. Stern, R. M. , and Colburn, H. S. (1985). “Lateral-position-based models of interaural discrimination,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 77, 753755.
11.
12. Trahiotis, C. , Bernstein, L. R. , and Akeroyd, M. A. (2001). “Manipulating the ‘straightness’ and ‘curvature’ of patterns of interaural cross-correlation affects listeners’ sensitivity to changes in interaural delay,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 321330.
12.
13. Trahiotis, C. , and Stern, R. M. (1989). “Lateralization of bands of noise: Effects of bandwidth and differences of interaural time and phase,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 86, 12851293.
13.
14. Yost, W. A. (1981). “Lateral position of sinusoids presented with interaural intensive and temporal differences,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 70, 397409.
14.
15. Yost, W. A. , and Dye, R. H. (1988). “Discrimination of interaural differences of level as a function of frequency,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 18461851.
http://aip.metastore.ingenta.com/content/asa/journal/jasa/129/1/10.1121/1.3528756
Loading
View: Figures

Figures

Image of FIG. 1.

Click to view

FIG. 1.

IID of the pointer (in decibels) required to match the intracranial position of the target as a function of its IID. Positive values of IID favored the left ear and negative values favored the right ear. The filled squares in the top, middle, and bottom panels represent the IID of the 500-Hz-centered noise pointer required to make a match when the 4-kHz-centered target stimulus was a SAM tone, a raised-sine, or a Gaussian band of noise, respectively. Each inset depicts an exemplar of the corresponding 4-kHz-centered stimulus. The filled circles in each panel represent the IID required to make a match when the target was the 500-Hz-centered noise and pointer was one of the afore-mentioned 4-kHz-centered stimuli. Error bars represent ±1 standard error of the mean. Unfilled circles represent predictions made with the regression lines calculated using the data represented by the filled squares but with the x and y axes “interchanged” (see text).

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/asa/journal/jasa/129/1/10.1121/1.3528756
2011-01-18
2014-04-24

Abstract

The purpose of this communication is to report the results of a study indicating that a given magnitude of interaural intensitive disparity (IID) produced a larger extent of laterality, as measured via an acoustic pointer, for stimuli centered at 4 kHz than for stimuli centered at 500 Hz. The data and their analysis, taken together, suggest that the findings reflect true across-frequency differences rather than being manifestations of response-related factors.

Loading

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/asa/journal/jasa/129/1/1.3528756.html;jsessionid=6159fu4be3nha.x-aip-live-01?itemId=/content/asa/journal/jasa/129/1/10.1121/1.3528756&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah&containerItemId=content/asa/journal/jasa
true
true
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
752b84549af89a08dbdd7fdb8b9568b5 journal.articlezxybnytfddd
Scitation: Lateralization produced by interaural intensitive disparities appears to be larger for high- vs low-frequency stimuli
http://aip.metastore.ingenta.com/content/asa/journal/jasa/129/1/10.1121/1.3528756
10.1121/1.3528756
SEARCH_EXPAND_ITEM