Skip to main content
banner image
No data available.
Please log in to see this content.
You have no subscription access to this content.
No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.
1. Alexander, J. M. , and Lutfi, R. A. (2004). “ Informational masking in hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners: Sensation level and decision weights,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 22342247.
2. Berg, B. G. (2004). “ A molecular description of profile analysis: Decision weights and internal noise,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115(2), 822829.
3. Buss, E. (2008). “ Across-channel interference in intensity discrimination: The role of practice and listening strategy,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123(1), 265272.
4. Durlach, N. I. , Mason, C. R. , Kidd, G. , Jr., Arbogast, T. L. , Colburn, H. S. , and Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2003). “ Note on informational masking (L),” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113(6), 29842987.
5. Gatehouse, S. , and Noble, W. (2004). “ The speech, spatial and qualities of hearing scale (SSQ),” Int. J. Audiol. 43(2), 8599.
6. Glasberg, B. R. , and Moore, B. C. J. (1990). “ Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data,” Hear. Res. 47(1–2), 103138.
7. Hawkey, D. J. C. , Amitay, S. , and Moore, D. R. (2004). “ Early and rapid perceptual learning,” Nat. Neurosci. 7(10), 10551056.
8. Jones, P. R. , Shub, D. E. , Moore, D. R. , and Amitay, S. (2013). “ Reduction of internal noise in auditory perceptual learning,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 133(2), 970981.
9. Kidd, G. , Jr., Mason, C. R. , Richards, V. M. , Gallun, F. J. , and Durlach, N. I. (2007). “ Informational masking,” in Auditory Perception of Sound Sources, edited by S. A. S. Christophe Micheyl and A. J. Oxenham (Springer Science+Business Media, New York), pp. 143189.
10. Lee, T. Y. , and Richards, V. M. (2011). “ Evaluation of similarity effects in informational masking,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 129(6), EL280EL285.
11. Lutfi, R. A. (1993). “ A model of auditory pattern analysis based on component-relative-entropy,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 94(2), 748758.
12. Neff, D. L. , and Callaghan, B. P. (1988). “ Effective properties of multicomponent simultaneous maskers under conditions of uncertainty,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83(5), 18331838.
13. Neff, D. L. , and Dethlefs, T. M. (1995). “ Individual differences in simultaneous masking with random-frequency, multicomponent maskers,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98(1), 125134.
14. Watson, C. S. , Wroton, H. W. , Kelly, W. J. , and Benbassat, C. A. (1975). “ Factors in the discrimination of tonal patterns. I. Component frequency, temporal position, and silent intervals,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 57(5), 11751185.
15. Wichmann, F. A. , and Hill, N. J. (2001). “ The psychometric function: I. fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit,” Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 63(8), 12931313.

Data & Media loading...


Article metrics loading...



Eight normal-hearing listeners practiced a tone-detection task in which a 1-kHz target was masked by a spectrally unpredictable multitone complex. Consistent learning was observed, with mean masking decreasing by 6.4 dB over five sessions (4500 trials). Reverse-correlation was used to estimate how listeners weighted each spectral region. Weight-vectors approximated the ideal more closely after practice, indicating that listeners were learning to attend selectively to the task relevant information. Once changes in weights were accounted for, no changes in internal noise (psychometric slope) were observed. It is concluded that this task elicits robust learning, which can be understood primarily as improved selective attention.


Full text loading...


Access Key

  • FFree Content
  • OAOpen Access Content
  • SSubscribed Content
  • TFree Trial Content
752b84549af89a08dbdd7fdb8b9568b5 journal.articlezxybnytfddd