Skip to main content

News about Scitation

In December 2016 Scitation will launch with a new design, enhanced navigation and a much improved user experience.

To ensure a smooth transition, from today, we are temporarily stopping new account registration and single article purchases. If you already have an account you can continue to use the site as normal.

For help or more information please visit our FAQs.

banner image
No data available.
Please log in to see this content.
You have no subscription access to this content.
No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.
/content/asa/journal/jasa/138/4/10.1121/1.4931439
1.
1. O. L. Railsback, “ Scale temperament as applied to piano tuning,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 9, 274 (1938).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1902056
2.
2. O. L. Railsback, “ A study of the tuning of pianos,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 10, 86 (1938).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1902080
3.
3. R. W. Young, “ Inharmonicity of plain wire piano strings,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 24, 267273 (1952).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1906888
4.
4. N. Fletcher, “ Normal vibration frequencies of a stiff piano string,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 36, 203209 (1964).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1918933
5.
5. R. Plomp and W. J. M. Levelt, “ Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 38, 548560 (1965).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1909741
6.
6. W. A. Sethares, “ Local consonance and the relationship between timbre and scale,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 94, 12181228 (1993).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.408175
7.
7. W. A. Sethares, Tuning, Timbre, Spectrum, Scale ( Springer, London, 2005).
8.
8. S. Dostrovsky, “ Early vibration theory: Physics and music in the seventeenth century,” Arch. Hist. Exact Sci. 14, 169218 (1975).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00327447
9.
9. O. H. Schuck and R. W. Young, “ Observations on the vibrations of piano strings,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 15, 111 (1943).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1916221
10.
10. N. H. Fletcher and T. D. Rossing, The Physics of Musical Instruments ( Springer-Verlag, New York, 1991), Chaps. 2 and 12.
11.
11. F. Rigaud, B. David, and L. Daudet, “ A parametric model and estimation techniques for the inharmonicity and tuning of the piano,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 133, 31073118 (2013).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4799806
12.
12. H. Hinrichsen, “ Entropy-based tuning of musical instruments,” Rev. Bras. Ens. Fís. 34, 2301 (2012).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1806-11172012000200004
13.
13. A. Kameoka and M. Kuriyagawa, “ Consonance theory part I: Consonance of dyads,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 45, 14511459 (1969).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1911623
14.
14. A. Kameoka and M. Kuriyagawa, “ Consonance theory part II: Consonance of complex tones and its calculation method,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 45, 14601469 (1969).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1911624
15.
15. E. Zwicker, B. Flottorp, and S. S. Stevens, “ Critical band width in loudness summation,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 29, 548557 (1957).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1908963
16.
16. J. R. Pierce, “ Attaining consonance in scales,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 40, 249 (1966).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1910051
17.
17. W. A. Sethares, “ Adaptive tunings for musical scales,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 96, 1018 (1994).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.410471
18.
18. D. E. Farrell, private communication (2015).
19.
19. R. N. Bracewell, The Fourier Transformation and its Applications, 2nd ed. ( McGraw-Hill, New York, 1978), Chaps. 2 and 4.
20.
20. R. E. Kirk, “ Tuning preferences for piano unison groups,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 31, 16441648 (1959).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1907673
21.
21. The reason for this seems to be that for a well-tuned piano, the difference in frequency between unison strings is much smaller than frequency differences associated with the stretch tuning.
22.
22. D. M. Koenig, Spectral Analysis of Musical Sounds with Emphasis on the Piano ( Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2015), Chap. 12.
23.
23. This approach for shifting the frequency of a partial is valid for all normal partials. It will not apply to longitudinal modes or so-called phantom partials (Refs. 26 and 27) which are known to be present in bass tones. Careful examination of the spectra, as in Fig. 2, allowed us to distinguish the longitudinal modes and phantom partials, and we found that they were all sufficiently weak and small enough in number that they had no effect on the calculated dissonance.
24.
24. This approach for shifting the partial frequencies should be accurate to order α in Eq. (2). Since α was typically of order 5 × 10−5, this approximation should be adequate.
25.
25. H. Fletcher and W. A. Munson, “ Loudness, its definition, measurement and calculation,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 5, 82108 (1933).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1915637
26.
26. J. H. A. Conklin, “ Design and tone in the mechanoacoustic piano. Part III. Piano strings and scale design,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 12861298 (1996).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.416017
27.
27. J. H. A. Conklin, “ Piano strings and ‘phantom’ partials,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102, 659 (1997).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.419713
http://aip.metastore.ingenta.com/content/asa/journal/jasa/138/4/10.1121/1.4931439
Loading
/content/asa/journal/jasa/138/4/10.1121/1.4931439
Loading

Data & Media loading...

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/asa/journal/jasa/138/4/10.1121/1.4931439
2015-10-23
2016-12-09

Abstract

The perceptual results of Plomp and Levelt [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. , 548–560 (1965)] for the sensory dissonance of a pair of pure tones are used to estimate the dissonance of pairs of piano tones. By using the spectra of tones measured for a real piano, the effect of the inharmonicity of the tones is included. This leads to a prediction for how the tuning of this piano should deviate from an ideal equal tempered scale so as to give the smallest sensory dissonance and hence give the most pleasing tuning. The results agree with the well known “Railsback stretch,” the average tuning curve produced by skilled piano technicians. The authors' analysis thus gives a quantitative explanation of the magnitude of the Railsback stretch in terms of the human perception of dissonance.

Loading

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/asa/journal/jasa/138/4/1.4931439.html;jsessionid=p8Ru8CO0GGqswoy-LTFpnlX0.x-aip-live-03?itemId=/content/asa/journal/jasa/138/4/10.1121/1.4931439&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah&containerItemId=content/asa/journal/jasa
true
true

Access Key

  • FFree Content
  • OAOpen Access Content
  • SSubscribed Content
  • TFree Trial Content
752b84549af89a08dbdd7fdb8b9568b5 journal.articlezxybnytfddd
/content/realmedia?fmt=ahah&adPositionList=
&advertTargetUrl=//oascentral.aip.org/RealMedia/ads/&sitePageValue=asadl.org/jasa/138/4/10.1121/1.4931439&pageURL=http://scitation.aip.org/content/asa/journal/jasa/138/4/10.1121/1.4931439'
Right1,Right2,Right3,